51精品视频

Tags
  • Innovation and Research
Features & Articles

Study Finds Difference Between Mindless and Distracted Eating

A woman eats popcorn while looking at her laptop
For many who are working from home during the COVID-19 crisis, kitchen or dining room tables have transformed into desks and makeshift laptop stations. That includes Carli Liguori, visiting instructor in the 51精品视频鈥檚 .

鈥淎ppropriately, I use my kitchen table as a standing desk,鈥 said Liguori, who is a registered dietitian and studies the behavioral aspect of food and how people choose to eat.

Long before COVID-19, Liguori was interested in studying the effects of distracted and mindless eating. Working from home or not, who among us hasn鈥檛 eaten lunch while checking email, snuck in a snack while driving or squeezed in a bite while on the phone?

Carli Liguori
鈥淓specially right now during the coronavirus outbreak, we鈥檙e grappling with both mindless and distracted eating. We鈥檙e mindlessly snacking on things because they just so happen to be there,鈥 said Liguori. 鈥淥n the other hand, many of us do prepare our meals with intention. But, when we eat it, a lot of times, our attention isn鈥檛 actually going into eating it. We鈥檙e distracted by answering emails or watching the news.鈥

Working and learning from home also creates less of a boundary for personal time, said Liguori, making that lunch hour or coffee break less defined.

鈥淎 lot of us have trouble putting work down. We鈥檙e working through lunch more so than we were before, and that may lead to even more distracted or mindless eating,鈥 said Liguori.

鈥淢indless eating occurs when you eat at a time when you were not intending to eat, whereas distracted eating occurs when you plan to eat but are also doing something else,鈥 she said.

Before the pandemic, Liguori conducted what she called 鈥渢he first study to draw attention to the difference to distracted and mindless eating鈥濃攁nd found there really is a difference.

The study: distracted vs. mindless eating

In the , published in the聽Journal of Nutrition earlier this year, Liguori found that not all forms of cognitive distraction result in the same food intake. Her results show that engaging in some forms of 鈥渄istracted鈥 eating may cause people to eat less, whereas engaging in 鈥渕indless鈥 eating may cause people to eat more.

The study was completed while Liguori was earning her master鈥檚 degree in food science and human nutrition at the University of Illinois with co-authors were Cassandra Nikolaus from Washington State University聽and Sharon M.聽Nickols-Richardson at the University of Illinois.聽

鈥淎t a time when obesity is a worldwide epidemic, we know that people are eating as a secondary activity more and more. It is something that we can all relate to,鈥 said Liguori.

The study involved a randomized controlled study of 119 healthy adults. Participants' food consumption was evaluated on two separate occasions: once while they engaged in distracted eating and on another day when they ate without distractions.

The distracted group ate their spinach and cheese quiche while playing a computer game in which they hit the spacebar whenever a specific series of numbers appeared on the screen. The non-distracted group ate their quiche without being asked to complete the task. After a rest period, the participants were offered a second snack of grapes聽and miniature chocolate chip cookies.

She said the researchers were 鈥渧ery surprised鈥 to observe that the distracted participants who completed the task ate significantly less quiche than their non-distracted counterparts. In addition, there was no difference between the groups in terms of who preferred cookies or grapes afterward. Based on prior research, they expected distracted eaters to eat more quiche and to make more unhealthy choices.聽

鈥淥ur results聽suggest there is a difference between the terms 鈥榤indless eating鈥 and 鈥榙istracted eating,鈥 which are often used interchangeably in the current literature,鈥 said Liguori. 鈥淲e believe there is a case to better differentiate these terms.鈥

However, before anyone celebrates the results as an excuse to eat in front of the TV, the study has several limitations with regards to its generalizability.

First, the participants were college-aged adults who may be healthier than the general population and are not necessarily a representative sample size. Second, all participants reported a low enjoyment of the quiche as a food choice. Third, the task that was selected in this research study may present a different level of cognitive load than other forms of distracted eating.聽

Doing the best we can

While we stay at home, Liguori said it鈥檚 important to pay attention to our food and think about the environment in which we鈥檙e eating.

鈥淲hen possible, mindfully prepare the foods you eat. If, inevitably, you find yourself eating distractedly, you are still eating the foods you intended to consume and getting those nutrients into your body,鈥 she said.

She added it鈥檚 also important to remind ourselves that 鈥渨e鈥檙e just doing the best we can.鈥
鈥淭here鈥檚 a lot going on in the world right now. There鈥檚 so much talk on social media about the 鈥楺uarantine Fifteen,鈥欌 she said. 鈥淭he most important priority is to keep you and your family healthy.鈥