51精品视频

a person in an MRI scanner
Features & Articles

How not to use brain scans in neuroscience

Tags
  • Innovation and Research
  • Graduate and professional students
  • Kenneth P. Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences

What does it take to know a person?

If you鈥檝e seen how a friend acts across different domains of their life, you might reasonably say you know who they are. Compare that to watching an interview with a celebrity 鈥 maybe you can claim some knowledge about them, but a single observation of a stranger can only tell you so much.

Yet a similar idea 鈥 that a lone snapshot of a brain can tell you about an individual鈥檚 personality or mental health 鈥 has been the basis of decades of neuroscience studies. That approach was punctured by earlier this year showing that scientists have massively underestimated how large such studies must be to produce reliable findings.

鈥淭he more we learn about who we are as people, the more we learn that, on average, we鈥檙e much more similar than we are different 鈥 and so understanding those differences is really challenging,鈥 said Brenden Tervo-Clemmens (A&S 鈥21G), now a postdoctoral fellow at Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School who co-led the multi-institutional research as a clinical psychology PhD student at 51精品视频.

At the center of the research is MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) brain scans. While invaluable for diagnosing brain conditions, they鈥檝e also been used by researchers to draw links between a person鈥檚 brain structure and aspects of their personality and mental health. Tervo-Clemmens and his colleagues call this technique brain-wide association scans, or BWAS, in a nod to 鈥淕WAS鈥 studies that attempt to decipher the often-tiny effects of genes from massive datasets (as seen in dubious science headlines announcing 鈥渁 gene for depression鈥 or 鈥渁 gene for intelligence鈥).

鈥淭he approach is similar: Here鈥檚 one profile of you biologically, how well can we determine the complexity of your human experience?鈥 Tervo-Clemmens said. 鈥淎nd the answer is, usually not very well.鈥

A typical study of the kind would include somewhere around 25 participants, due in part to the high cost of running scans. But, Tervo-Clemmens and his colleagues showed, scientists would need to scan the brains of more than 1,000 to be confident that the connections they find aren鈥檛 just a statistical mirage.

Simulating science

Reaching that conclusion required getting a far broader view of the field than was possible until recently. Along with colleagues at a number of institutions as well as his advisor, 51精品视频 Professor of Psychiatry , Tervo-Clemmens combined three recent publicly available studies that together included MRI data from around 50,000 participants.

Using this massive body of information, the team simulated the process of science, selecting groups of the scans at random as if they were patients recruited to a study. By repeating that process over and over, the researchers could figure out how likely it is that any given number of scans would produce a misleading result simply due to chance 鈥 and how many participants it takes for a study to be reliable.

Not every investigation requires 1,000 brain scans, they showed. 鈥淚f the goal is just to understand something like the general organization of the brain, we sometimes only need 10 to 20 participants to do that,鈥 Tervo-Clemmens said. It鈥檚 only because a single brain scan reveals so little about a person鈥檚 personality and mental health that researchers need a massive amount of data before these complex traits begin to reliably stand out from the statistical noise.

Amplifying that problem is a well-known bug in 21st century science: Researchers are often rewarded for publishing results that show exciting new connections, rather than less glamorous findings suggesting the absence of a connection. The latter results are less likely to be published and more likely to languish on a hard drive. So not only are small imaging studies more likely to 鈥渄iscover鈥 a link that isn鈥檛 actually there, but those same misleading studies also receive a disproportionate amount of attention.

Tervo-Clemmens is quick to note that the Nature paper wasn鈥檛 intended to call out the whole field. Neuroscientists and psychologists have successfully tackled questions about personality and mental health using a variety of other techniques. And brain scans on their own are very effective for diagnosing conditions and mapping out the broader picture of how brains work. It鈥檚 when scientists combine the two, reducing the complexities of a person into a single image, that they fall short.

鈥淲e can count on less than a hand the number of these studies that have held up under scrutiny and are really driving treatment,鈥 he said. 鈥淚n my own area, one study might show that increased function of a particular brain region is related to more symptoms, but you can find, almost without question, another study showing the opposite effect.鈥

Although he now focuses mostly on psychiatric and substance-use disorders in adolescents, Tervo-Clemmens doesn鈥檛 quite fit into any one box as a researcher. 鈥淚鈥檓 kind of a psychologist, and I鈥檓 kind of a statistician, and I鈥檓 kind of a neuroscientist,鈥 he said. It鈥檚 that perspective, he explains, that helps him do the kind of broad critical research like this current study, along with his boundary-crossing education at 51精品视频.

He saw patients as a PhD student in clinical psychology while also training in cross-disciplinary programs like , experiences he credits as encouraging breadth in research. 鈥淚 think that level of integration is what makes 51精品视频 so awesome as a graduate student,鈥 he said.

Growing pains

The result was a study that鈥檚 already produced a stir among other scientists. An instant classic, the paper and its pre-publication version have by more than 250 other scholarly works.

So where does that leave the field?

First, Tervo-Clemmens said, it鈥檚 necessary to re-examine the smaller studies of the past to see if their results hold up to further examination. As for future research, one solution would be to simply supersize brain-scan studies of complex behavior so they stand up to statistical scrutiny. But there鈥檚 another possible way forward, where researchers find ways to study patients over time and across contexts to get a fuller sense of their identities.

鈥淲e need to be aligning our research to how we generally think and understand human beings,鈥 said Tervo-Clemmens. 鈥淭hat鈥檚 a challenge of cost and economy. But I also think it鈥檚 one that will ultimately be worth it.鈥

It鈥檚 like growing pains for a line of research that鈥檚 only a few decades old: Stressful and full of uncertainty, but also a sign that the field is heading in new and exciting directions.

Image from Getty